1.
Introduction
Early generative
grammar work claimed that English imperatives disallow the passive forms
syntactically (cf. Lees 1964), based on facts like those in (*Be helped by
Jill) above. However, later pragmatic and descriptive work found that this
restriction is far from absolute (cf. Bolinger 1997; Davies 1986). – 239
2.
English imperatives and their four features
In English, the
‘imperative’ refers to a construction which occurs only in main clause,
normally has no grammatical subject and contains the verb in its basic form. –
240
[Takahashi]
characterized English imperatives in terms of four essential features, hypotheticality, non-past, second person
and force exertion. – 240
Force Exertion:
the degree of (directive) force that the speaker is applying (at the utterance
time of an imperative) toward the addressee’s carrying out the action. – 240
3.
A cognitive (image-schema) model of imperatives
3.1
Action chain model
i. The imperative
is comprised of two separate (though interrelated) subevents, which combine to
from a dynamic chain of action;
ii. The speaker
and the addressee both participate in this action chain as indispensable
entities – the speaker as head, the addressee as a second entity engaged in
further action;
iii. The
imperative makes explicit only the addressee’s action and leaves implicit the
two key entities; nor does it overtly code the application of directive force.
– 242
3.2
Canonical event model
The simplest
transitive clause contains two participants which play semantic roles, agent
and patient, respectively. These participants are engaged in a kind of
energetic interaction in a specific conceptual domain called a ‘setting’ chosen
to be highlighted by a conceptualizer. A transitive clause such as John ate the apple in the kitchen, where
John acts as an agent and participates in an energetic interaction of eating an
apple which is patient in the ‘kitchen’ setting. – 243
To obtain a Canonical
Event Model of the imperative, let us specify the setting and the semantic
roles of participants. As for the setting, the first subevent is restricted
to an extremely narrow speech situation, the here-and-now of speaking, only
populated by the speaker and addressee. It would be reasonable then to analyze
this subevent as occurring in a deictic
setting. On the other hand, the second subevent (or the addressee’s action) is
hypothetical is native, regardless of whether or not the addressee’s act will
be reanalyzed in the objective world. For this reason, the second subevent may
be treated as taking place in a hypothetical
setting (or mental space). – 243
The speaker can
be analyzed in terms of cause-agent,
and the addressee in terms of cause-agent.
The rationale behind this treatment is that in the prototypical scene of an
imperative, the addressee is triggered
to act or undergo a substantial change in state by the utterance of an
imperative – 244
The
prototype IMPERATIVE
i. The speakers
exerts a high (near [+1]) directive force in deictic setting
towards the addressee, who will thereby perform
an action in hypothetical setting.
ii. The speaker
plays a semantic role of causer-like
agent, and the addressee cause-like
agent. – 244
The
schematic IMPERATIVE
i. The speaker
exerts a varying degree of force (ranging from [+1] to [-1] in deictic setting
toward the addressee, who will thereby be engaged in a certain situation in
hypothetical setting.
ii. The speaker
and addressee may play an agentive as well as non-agentive role. – 245
(A) Imperatives
are atypical when the speaker’s force in non-directive (or mixed with other
kinds of force). […] (B) When the subject is a non-agent such as an
experiencer, theme or patient or when the understood subject is generic as in Shake before using, the imperative
progressively deviates from its norm. (C) Events (highly dynamic situations)
better fit the image schema of prototype imperatives than states as in Be sick (and they’ll put you in bed). -
246
4.
English imperatives and passives
It is well
established that an active clause more readily passivies if it is more
transitive, i.e. when the patient is directly affected by the activity in
question. – 247
The passive
critically involves an agent (or something close to it such as an experiencer),
which is invariably defocused either syntactically or lexically. Second, the
passive with be is in principle more
static than its active counterpart, alhtough this does not mean that the be-passive only refers to a state.
Third, the subject tends to be a patient. – 247
The
prototype passive
i. The subject is
directly affected by an external agent.
ii. The subject plays the semantic role of patient. – 247
The subject’s
patienthood, the defocused participant’s agency and overall affectedness
normally go hand in hand. One almost automatically follows from another, since
prototypical patient is a participant absorbing the energy transmitted from
without and thereby undergoing a change in state. – 248
Hypothesis: The passive
construction does not clash with the imperative syntactically but on conceptual
grounds, i.e., the clash occurs between prototypes. – 249
5.
Japanese imperatives and passives
No comments:
Post a Comment